<p><SPAN name="link2HCH0008" id="link2HCH0008"></SPAN></p>
<h2> CHAPTER VIII. INSTINCT. </h2>
<p>Instincts comparable with habits, but different in their<br/>
origin—Instincts graduated—Aphides and ants—Instincts<br/>
variable—Domestic instincts, their origin—Natural instincts of<br/>
the cuckoo, molothrus, ostrich, and parasitic bees—Slave-making<br/>
ants—Hive-bee, its cell-making instinct—Changes of instinct and<br/>
structure not necessarily simultaneous—Difficulties of the theory of<br/>
the Natural Selection of instincts—Neuter or sterile insects—Summary.<br/></p>
<p>Many instincts are so wonderful that their development will probably
appear to the reader a difficulty sufficient to overthrow my whole theory.
I may here premise, that I have nothing to do with the origin of the
mental powers, any more than I have with that of life itself. We are
concerned only with the diversities of instinct and of the other mental
faculties in animals of the same class.</p>
<p>I will not attempt any definition of instinct. It would be easy to show
that several distinct mental actions are commonly embraced by this term;
but every one understands what is meant, when it is said that instinct
impels the cuckoo to migrate and to lay her eggs in other birds' nests. An
action, which we ourselves require experience to enable us to perform,
when performed by an animal, more especially by a very young one, without
experience, and when performed by many individuals in the same way,
without their knowing for what purpose it is performed, is usually said to
be instinctive. But I could show that none of these characters are
universal. A little dose of judgment or reason, as Pierre Huber expresses
it, often comes into play, even with animals low in the scale of nature.</p>
<p>Frederick Cuvier and several of the older metaphysicians have compared
instinct with habit. This comparison gives, I think, an accurate notion of
the frame of mind under which an instinctive action is performed, but not
necessarily of its origin. How unconsciously many habitual actions are
performed, indeed not rarely in direct opposition to our conscious will!
yet they may be modified by the will or reason. Habits easily become
associated with other habits, with certain periods of time and states of
the body. When once acquired, they often remain constant throughout life.
Several other points of resemblance between instincts and habits could be
pointed out. As in repeating a well-known song, so in instincts, one
action follows another by a sort of rhythm; if a person be interrupted in
a song, or in repeating anything by rote, he is generally forced to go
back to recover the habitual train of thought: so P. Huber found it was
with a caterpillar, which makes a very complicated hammock; for if he took
a caterpillar which had completed its hammock up to, say, the sixth stage
of construction, and put it into a hammock completed up only to the third
stage, the caterpillar simply re-performed the fourth, fifth, and sixth
stages of construction. If, however, a caterpillar were taken out of a
hammock made up, for instance, to the third stage, and were put into one
finished up to the sixth stage, so that much of its work was already done
for it, far from deriving any benefit from this, it was much embarrassed,
and, in order to complete its hammock, seemed forced to start from the
third stage, where it had left off, and thus tried to complete the already
finished work.</p>
<p>If we suppose any habitual action to become inherited—and it can be
shown that this does sometimes happen—then the resemblance between
what originally was a habit and an instinct becomes so close as not to be
distinguished. If Mozart, instead of playing the pianoforte at three years
old with wonderfully little practice, had played a tune with no practice
at all, be might truly be said to have done so instinctively. But it would
be a serious error to suppose that the greater number of instincts have
been acquired by habit in one generation, and then transmitted by
inheritance to succeeding generations. It can be clearly shown that the
most wonderful instincts with which we are acquainted, namely, those of
the hive-bee and of many ants, could not possibly have been acquired by
habit.</p>
<p>It will be universally admitted that instincts are as important as
corporeal structures for the welfare of each species, under its present
conditions of life. Under changed conditions of life, it is at least
possible that slight modifications of instinct might be profitable to a
species; and if it can be shown that instincts do vary ever so little,
then I can see no difficulty in natural selection preserving and
continually accumulating variations of instinct to any extent that was
profitable. It is thus, as I believe, that all the most complex and
wonderful instincts have originated. As modifications of corporeal
structure arise from, and are increased by, use or habit, and are
diminished or lost by disuse, so I do not doubt it has been with
instincts. But I believe that the effects of habit are in many cases of
subordinate importance to the effects of the natural selection of what may
be called spontaneous variations of instincts;—that is of variations
produced by the same unknown causes which produce slight deviations of
bodily structure.</p>
<p>No complex instinct can possibly be produced through natural selection,
except by the slow and gradual accumulation of numerous, slight, yet
profitable, variations. Hence, as in the case of corporeal structures, we
ought to find in nature, not the actual transitional gradations by which
each complex instinct has been acquired—for these could be found
only in the lineal ancestors of each species—but we ought to find in
the collateral lines of descent some evidence of such gradations; or we
ought at least to be able to show that gradations of some kind are
possible; and this we certainly can do. I have been surprised to find,
making allowance for the instincts of animals having been but little
observed, except in Europe and North America, and for no instinct being
known among extinct species, how very generally gradations, leading to the
most complex instincts, can be discovered. Changes of instinct may
sometimes be facilitated by the same species having different instincts at
different periods of life, or at different seasons of the year, or when
placed under different circumstances, etc.; in which case either the one
or the other instinct might be preserved by natural selection. And such
instances of diversity of instinct in the same species can be shown to
occur in nature.</p>
<p>Again, as in the case of corporeal structure, and conformably to my
theory, the instinct of each species is good for itself, but has never, as
far as we can judge, been produced for the exclusive good of others. One
of the strongest instances of an animal apparently performing an action
for the sole good of another, with which I am acquainted, is that of
aphides voluntarily yielding, as was first observed by Huber, their sweet
excretion to ants: that they do so voluntarily, the following facts show.
I removed all the ants from a group of about a dozen aphides on a
dock-plant, and prevented their attendance during several hours. After
this interval, I felt sure that the aphides would want to excrete. I
watched them for some time through a lens, but not one excreted; I then
tickled and stroked them with a hair in the same manner, as well as I
could, as the ants do with their antennae; but not one excreted.
Afterwards, I allowed an ant to visit them, and it immediately seemed, by
its eager way of running about to be well aware what a rich flock it had
discovered; it then began to play with its antennae on the abdomen first
of one aphis and then of another; and each, as soon as it felt the
antennae, immediately lifted up its abdomen and excreted a limpid drop of
sweet juice, which was eagerly devoured by the ant. Even the quite young
aphides behaved in this manner, showing that the action was instinctive,
and not the result of experience. It is certain, from the observations of
Huber, that the aphides show no dislike to the ants: if the latter be not
present they are at last compelled to eject their excretion. But as the
excretion is extremely viscid, it is no doubt a convenience to the aphides
to have it removed; therefore probably they do not excrete solely for the
good of the ants. Although there is no evidence that any animal performs
an action for the exclusive good of another species, yet each tries to
take advantage of the instincts of others, as each takes advantage of the
weaker bodily structure of other species. So again certain instincts
cannot be considered as absolutely perfect; but as details on this and
other such points are not indispensable, they may be here passed over.</p>
<p>As some degree of variation in instincts under a state of nature, and the
inheritance of such variations, are indispensable for the action of
natural selection, as many instances as possible ought to be given; but
want of space prevents me. I can only assert that instincts certainly do
vary—for instance, the migratory instinct, both in extent and
direction, and in its total loss. So it is with the nests of birds, which
vary partly in dependence on the situations chosen, and on the nature and
temperature of the country inhabited, but often from causes wholly unknown
to us. Audubon has given several remarkable cases of differences in the
nests of the same species in the northern and southern United States. Why,
it has been asked, if instinct be variable, has it not granted to the bee
"the ability to use some other material when wax was deficient?" But what
other natural material could bees use? They will work, as I have seen,
with wax hardened with vermilion or softened with lard. Andrew Knight
observed that his bees, instead of laboriously collecting propolis, used a
cement of wax and turpentine, with which he had covered decorticated
trees. It has lately been shown that bees, instead of searching for
pollen, will gladly use a very different substance, namely, oatmeal. Fear
of any particular enemy is certainly an instinctive quality, as may be
seen in nestling birds, though it is strengthened by experience, and by
the sight of fear of the same enemy in other animals. The fear of man is
slowly acquired, as I have elsewhere shown, by the various animals which
inhabit desert islands; and we see an instance of this, even in England,
in the greater wildness of all our large birds in comparison with our
small birds; for the large birds have been most persecuted by man. We may
safely attribute the greater wildness of our large birds to this cause;
for in uninhabited islands large birds are not more fearful than small;
and the magpie, so wary in England, is tame in Norway, as is the hooded
crow in Egypt.</p>
<p>That the mental qualities of animals of the same kind, born in a state of
nature, vary much, could be shown by many facts. Several cases could also
be adduced of occasional and strange habits in wild animals, which, if
advantageous to the species, might have given rise, through natural
selection, to new instincts. But I am well aware that these general
statements, without the facts in detail, can produce but a feeble effect
on the reader's mind. I can only repeat my assurance, that I do not speak
without good evidence.</p>
<p>INHERITED CHANGES OF HABIT OR INSTINCT IN DOMESTICATED ANIMALS.</p>
<p>The possibility, or even probability, of inherited variations of instinct
in a state of nature will be strengthened by briefly considering a few
cases under domestication. We shall thus be enabled to see the part which
habit and the selection of so-called spontaneous variations have played in
modifying the mental qualities of our domestic animals. It is notorious
how much domestic animals vary in their mental qualities. With cats, for
instance, one naturally takes to catching rats, and another mice, and
these tendencies are known to be inherited. One cat, according to Mr. St.
John, always brought home game birds, another hares or rabbits, and
another hunted on marshy ground and almost nightly caught woodcocks or
snipes. A number of curious and authentic instances could be given of
various shades of disposition and taste, and likewise of the oddest
tricks, associated with certain frames of mind or periods of time. But let
us look to the familiar case of the breeds of dogs: it cannot be doubted
that young pointers (I have myself seen striking instances) will sometimes
point and even back other dogs the very first time that they are taken
out; retrieving is certainly in some degree inherited by retrievers; and a
tendency to run round, instead of at, a flock of sheep, by shepherd-dogs.
I cannot see that these actions, performed without experience by the
young, and in nearly the same manner by each individual, performed with
eager delight by each breed, and without the end being known—for the
young pointer can no more know that he points to aid his master, than the
white butterfly knows why she lays her eggs on the leaf of the cabbage—I
cannot see that these actions differ essentially from true instincts. If
we were to behold one kind of wolf, when young and without any training,
as soon as it scented its prey, stand motionless like a statue, and then
slowly crawl forward with a peculiar gait; and another kind of wolf
rushing round, instead of at, a herd of deer, and driving them to a
distant point, we should assuredly call these actions instinctive.
Domestic instincts, as they may be called, are certainly far less fixed
than natural instincts; but they have been acted on by far less rigorous
selection, and have been transmitted for an incomparably shorter period,
under less fixed conditions of life.</p>
<p>How strongly these domestic instincts, habits, and dispositions are
inherited, and how curiously they become mingled, is well shown when
different breeds of dogs are crossed. Thus it is known that a cross with a
bull-dog has affected for many generations the courage and obstinacy of
greyhounds; and a cross with a greyhound has given to a whole family of
shepherd-dogs a tendency to hunt hares. These domestic instincts, when
thus tested by crossing, resemble natural instincts, which in a like
manner become curiously blended together, and for a long period exhibit
traces of the instincts of either parent: for example, Le Roy describes a
dog, whose great-grandfather was a wolf, and this dog showed a trace of
its wild parentage only in one way, by not coming in a straight line to
his master, when called.</p>
<p>Domestic instincts are sometimes spoken of as actions which have become
inherited solely from long-continued and compulsory habit, but this is not
true. No one would ever have thought of teaching, or probably could have
taught, the tumbler-pigeon to tumble—an action which, as I have
witnessed, is performed by young birds, that have never seen a pigeon
tumble. We may believe that some one pigeon showed a slight tendency to
this strange habit, and that the long-continued selection of the best
individuals in successive generations made tumblers what they now are; and
near Glasgow there are house-tumblers, as I hear from Mr. Brent, which
cannot fly eighteen inches high without going head over heels. It may be
doubted whether any one would have thought of training a dog to point, had
not some one dog naturally shown a tendency in this line; and this is
known occasionally to happen, as I once saw, in a pure terrier: the act of
pointing is probably, as many have thought, only the exaggerated pause of
an animal preparing to spring on its prey. When the first tendency to
point was once displayed, methodical selection and the inherited effects
of compulsory training in each successive generation would soon complete
the work; and unconscious selection is still in progress, as each man
tries to procure, without intending to improve the breed, dogs which stand
and hunt best. On the other hand, habit alone in some cases has sufficed;
hardly any animal is more difficult to tame than the young of the wild
rabbit; scarcely any animal is tamer than the young of the tame rabbit;
but I can hardly suppose that domestic rabbits have often been selected
for tameness alone; so that we must attribute at least the greater part of
the inherited change from extreme wildness to extreme tameness, to habit
and long-continued close confinement.</p>
<p>Natural instincts are lost under domestication: a remarkable instance of
this is seen in those breeds of fowls which very rarely or never become
"broody," that is, never wish to sit on their eggs. Familiarity alone
prevents our seeing how largely and how permanently the minds of our
domestic animals have been modified. It is scarcely possible to doubt that
the love of man has become instinctive in the dog. All wolves, foxes,
jackals and species of the cat genus, when kept tame, are most eager to
attack poultry, sheep and pigs; and this tendency has been found incurable
in dogs which have been brought home as puppies from countries such as
Tierra del Fuego and Australia, where the savages do not keep these
domestic animals. How rarely, on the other hand, do our civilised dogs,
even when quite young, require to be taught not to attack poultry, sheep,
and pigs! No doubt they occasionally do make an attack, and are then
beaten; and if not cured, they are destroyed; so that habit and some
degree of selection have probably concurred in civilising by inheritance
our dogs. On the other hand, young chickens have lost wholly by habit,
that fear of the dog and cat which no doubt was originally instinctive in
them, for I am informed by Captain Hutton that the young chickens of the
parent stock, the Gallus bankiva, when reared in India under a hen, are at
first excessively wild. So it is with young pheasants reared in England
under a hen. It is not that chickens have lost all fear, but fear only of
dogs and cats, for if the hen gives the danger chuckle they will run (more
especially young turkeys) from under her and conceal themselves in the
surrounding grass or thickets; and this is evidently done for the
instinctive purpose of allowing, as we see in wild ground-birds, their
mother to fly away. But this instinct retained by our chickens has become
useless under domestication, for the mother-hen has almost lost by disuse
the power of flight.</p>
<p>Hence, we may conclude that under domestication instincts have been
acquired and natural instincts have been lost, partly by habit and partly
by man selecting and accumulating, during successive generations, peculiar
mental habits and actions, which at first appeared from what we must in
our ignorance call an accident. In some cases compulsory habit alone has
sufficed to produce inherited mental changes; in other cases compulsory
habit has done nothing, and all has been the result of selection, pursued
both methodically and unconsciously; but in most cases habit and selection
have probably concurred.</p>
<p>SPECIAL INSTINCTS.</p>
<p>We shall, perhaps, best understand how instincts in a state of nature have
become modified by selection by considering a few cases. I will select
only three, namely, the instinct which leads the cuckoo to lay her eggs in
other birds' nests; the slave-making instinct of certain ants; and the
cell-making power of the hive-bee: these two latter instincts have
generally and justly been ranked by naturalists as the most wonderful of
all known instincts.</p>
<p>INSTINCTS OF THE CUCKOO.</p>
<p>It is supposed by some naturalists that the more immediate cause of the
instinct of the cuckoo is that she lays her eggs, not daily, but at
intervals of two or three days; so that, if she were to make her own nest
and sit on her own eggs, those first laid would have to be left for some
time unincubated or there would be eggs and young birds of different ages
in the same nest. If this were the case the process of laying and hatching
might be inconveniently long, more especially as she migrates at a very
early period; and the first hatched young would probably have to be fed by
the male alone. But the American cuckoo is in this predicament, for she
makes her own nest and has eggs and young successively hatched, all at the
same time. It has been both asserted and denied that the American cuckoo
occasionally lays her eggs in other birds' nests; but I have lately heard
from Dr. Merrill, of Iowa, that he once found in Illinois a young cuckoo,
together with a young jay in the nest of a blue jay (Garrulus cristatus);
and as both were nearly full feathered, there could be no mistake in their
identification. I could also give several instances of various birds which
have been known occasionally to lay their eggs in other birds' nests. Now
let us suppose that the ancient progenitor of our European cuckoo had the
habits of the American cuckoo, and that she occasionally laid an egg in
another bird's nest. If the old bird profited by this occasional habit
through being enabled to emigrate earlier or through any other cause; or
if the young were made more vigorous by advantage being taken of the
mistaken instinct of another species than when reared by their own mother,
encumbered as she could hardly fail to be by having eggs and young of
different ages at the same time, then the old birds or the fostered young
would gain an advantage. And analogy would lead us to believe that the
young thus reared would be apt to follow by inheritance the occasional and
aberrant habit of their mother, and in their turn would be apt to lay
their eggs in other birds' nests, and thus be more successful in rearing
their young. By a continued process of this nature, I believe that the
strange instinct of our cuckoo has been generated. It has, also recently
been ascertained on sufficient evidence, by Adolf Muller, that the cuckoo
occasionally lays her eggs on the bare ground, sits on them and feeds her
young. This rare event is probably a case of reversion to the long-lost,
aboriginal instinct of nidification.</p>
<p>It has been objected that I have not noticed other related instincts and
adaptations of structure in the cuckoo, which are spoken of as necessarily
co-ordinated. But in all cases, speculation on an instinct known to us
only in a single species, is useless, for we have hitherto had no facts to
guide us. Until recently the instincts of the European and of the
non-parasitic American cuckoo alone were known; now, owing to Mr. Ramsay's
observations, we have learned something about three Australian species,
which lay their eggs in other birds' nests. The chief points to be
referred to are three: first, that the common cuckoo, with rare
exceptions, lays only one egg in a nest, so that the large and voracious
young bird receives ample food. Secondly, that the eggs are remarkably
small, not exceeding those of the skylark—a bird about one-fourth as
large as the cuckoo. That the small size of the egg is a real case of
adaptation we may infer from the fact of the mon-parasitic American cuckoo
laying full-sized eggs. Thirdly, that the young cuckoo, soon after birth,
has the instinct, the strength and a properly shaped back for ejecting its
foster-brothers, which then perish from cold and hunger. This has been
boldly called a beneficent arrangement, in order that the young cuckoo may
get sufficient food, and that its foster-brothers may perish before they
had acquired much feeling!</p>
<p>Turning now to the Australian species: though these birds generally lay
only one egg in a nest, it is not rare to find two and even three eggs in
the same nest. In the bronze cuckoo the eggs vary greatly in size, from
eight to ten lines in length. Now, if it had been of an advantage to this
species to have laid eggs even smaller than those now laid, so as to have
deceived certain foster-parents, or, as is more probable, to have been
hatched within a shorter period (for it is asserted that there is a
relation between the size of eggs and the period of their incubation),
then there is no difficulty in believing that a race or species might have
been formed which would have laid smaller and smaller eggs; for these
would have been more safely hatched and reared. Mr. Ramsay remarks that
two of the Australian cuckoos, when they lay their eggs in an open nest,
manifest a decided preference for nests containing eggs similar in colour
to their own. The European species apparently manifests some tendency
towards a similar instinct, but not rarely departs from it, as is shown by
her laying her dull and pale-coloured eggs in the nest of the
hedge-warbler with bright greenish-blue eggs. Had our cuckoo invariably
displayed the above instinct, it would assuredly have been added to those
which it is assumed must all have been acquired together. The eggs of the
Australian bronze cuckoo vary, according to Mr. Ramsay, to an
extraordinary degree in colour; so that in this respect, as well as in
size, natural selection might have secured and fixed any advantageous
variation.</p>
<p>In the case of the European cuckoo, the offspring of the foster-parents
are commonly ejected from the nest within three days after the cuckoo is
hatched; and as the latter at this age is in a most helpless condition,
Mr. Gould was formerly inclined to believe that the act of ejection was
performed by the foster-parents themselves. But he has now received a
trustworthy account of a young cuckoo which was actually seen, while still
blind and not able even to hold up its own head, in the act of ejecting
its foster-brothers. One of these was replaced in the nest by the
observer, and was again thrown out. With respect to the means by which
this strange and odious instinct was acquired, if it were of great
importance for the young cuckoo, as is probably the case, to receive as
much food as possible soon after birth, I can see no special difficulty in
its having gradually acquired, during successive generations, the blind
desire, the strength, and structure necessary for the work of ejection;
for those cuckoos which had such habits and structure best developed would
be the most securely reared. The first step towards the acquisition of the
proper instinct might have been mere unintentional restlessness on the
part of the young bird, when somewhat advanced in age and strength; the
habit having been afterwards improved, and transmitted to an earlier age.
I can see no more difficulty in this than in the unhatched young of other
birds acquiring the instinct to break through their own shells; or than in
young snakes acquiring in their upper jaws, as Owen has remarked, a
transitory sharp tooth for cutting through the tough egg-shell. For if
each part is liable to individual variations at all ages, and the
variations tend to be inherited at a corresponding or earlier age—propositions
which cannot be disputed—then the instincts and structure of the
young could be slowly modified as surely as those of the adult; and both
cases must stand or fall together with the whole theory of natural
selection.</p>
<p>Some species of Molothrus, a widely distinct genus of American birds,
allied to our starlings, have parasitic habits like those of the cuckoo;
and the species present an interesting gradation in the perfection of
their instincts. The sexes of Molothrus badius are stated by an excellent
observer, Mr. Hudson, sometimes to live promiscuously together in flocks,
and sometimes to pair. They either build a nest of their own or seize on
one belonging to some other bird, occasionally throwing out the nestlings
of the stranger. They either lay their eggs in the nest thus appropriated,
or oddly enough build one for themselves on the top of it. They usually
sit on their own eggs and rear their own young; but Mr. Hudson says it is
probable that they are occasionally parasitic, for he has seen the young
of this species following old birds of a distinct kind and clamouring to
be fed by them. The parasitic habits of another species of Molothrus, the
M. bonariensis, are much more highly developed than those of the last, but
are still far from perfect. This bird, as far as it is known, invariably
lays its eggs in the nests of strangers; but it is remarkable that several
together sometimes commence to build an irregular untidy nest of their
own, placed in singular ill-adapted situations, as on the leaves of a
large thistle. They never, however, as far as Mr. Hudson has ascertained,
complete a nest for themselves. They often lay so many eggs—from
fifteen to twenty—in the same foster-nest, that few or none can
possibly be hatched. They have, moreover, the extraordinary habit of
pecking holes in the eggs, whether of their own species or of their foster
parents, which they find in the appropriated nests. They drop also many
eggs on the bare ground, which are thus wasted. A third species, the M.
pecoris of North America, has acquired instincts as perfect as those of
the cuckoo, for it never lays more than one egg in a foster-nest, so that
the young bird is securely reared. Mr. Hudson is a strong disbeliever in
evolution, but he appears to have been so much struck by the imperfect
instincts of the Molothrus bonariensis that he quotes my words, and asks,
"Must we consider these habits, not as especially endowed or created
instincts, but as small consequences of one general law, namely,
transition?"</p>
<p>Various birds, as has already been remarked, occasionally lay their eggs
in the nests of other birds. This habit is not very uncommon with the
Gallinaceae, and throws some light on the singular instinct of the
ostrich. In this family several hen birds unite and lay first a few eggs
in one nest and then in another; and these are hatched by the males. This
instinct may probably be accounted for by the fact of the hens laying a
large number of eggs, but, as with the cuckoo, at intervals of two or
three days. The instinct, however, of the American ostrich, as in the case
of the Molothrus bonariensis, has not as yet been perfected; for a
surprising number of eggs lie strewed over the plains, so that in one
day's hunting I picked up no less than twenty lost and wasted eggs.</p>
<p>Many bees are parasitic, and regularly lay their eggs in the nests of
other kinds of bees. This case is more remarkable than that of the cuckoo;
for these bees have not only had their instincts but their structure
modified in accordance with their parasitic habits; for they do not
possess the pollen-collecting apparatus which would have been
indispensable if they had stored up food for their own young. Some species
of Sphegidae (wasp-like insects) are likewise parasitic; and M. Fabre has
lately shown good reason for believing that, although the Tachytes nigra
generally makes its own burrow and stores it with paralysed prey for its
own larvae, yet that, when this insect finds a burrow already made and
stored by another sphex, it takes advantage of the prize, and becomes for
the occasion parasitic. In this case, as with that of the Molothrus or
cuckoo, I can see no difficulty in natural selection making an occasional
habit permanent, if of advantage to the species, and if the insect whose
nest and stored food are feloniously appropriated, be not thus
exterminated.</p>
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />