<h2><SPAN name="CHAPTER_VIII" id="CHAPTER_VIII">CHAPTER VIII</SPAN></h2>
<p class="center">CRAB-SPIDERS. MIMICRY</p>
<p><span class="smcap">All</span> spiders can spin, but by no means all use
that power to entrap their prey. Many have no
settled abode or resting place except perhaps for a
short time when they are rearing their young. Among
these roving tribes, there are three groups which may
engage our attention for a time—the Crab-spiders
(Thomisidae), the Wolf-spiders (Lycosidae) and the
Jumping spiders (Attidae).</p>
<p>Crab-spiders are seldom seen by the ordinary
observer, for their habits do not bring them prominently
into notice, and many of them are of
small size. They are well named, for there is something
exceedingly crab-like in their appearance and<span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_53" id="Page_53">[Pg 53]</SPAN></span>
in their actions. Their body is generally broad and
flattened, and their legs, instead of being arranged
fore and aft, like those of most spiders, extend more
or less laterally, and though they can move pretty
actively in any direction their normal method of
progression is sideways. Then again, when frightened
they cramp their legs up under their bodies in a most
crab-like fashion and “sham dead.”</p>
<div class="figcenter"><SPAN name="f6" id="f6" /> <ANTIMG src="images/i_064.png" width-obs="300" height-obs="242" alt="Fig. 6. A Crab-spider." /> <p class="caption">Fig. 6. A Crab-spider (<i>Thomisidae</i>), × 3.</p> </div>
<p>We saw some of these spiders on the iron railing,
but their real haunts are among grass and herbage or
upon the trunks of trees. Some are true rovers,
hunting their prey by day and camping out wherever
they happen to find themselves at night. Their
methods are without guile—except that they approach
their victims warily; their trust is in rapidity of<span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_54" id="Page_54">[Pg 54]</SPAN></span>
action and superior strength. But other crab-spiders
lead a less strenuous life; their habit is to lurk in
moss, lichen, or flowers till an insect draws near enough
to be seized without any great expenditure of energy.</p>
<p>Now in the case of some of these spiders the
chance of obtaining a meal is very greatly increased
by a remarkable similarity of coloration between the
spider and its usual hunting ground. The spider’s
object is to remain invisible, and concealment is
obviously more easy if its colour matches that of its
environment. To a greater or less extent this
protective coloration as it is called prevails universally:—spiders
are seldom conspicuous objects
among their usual surroundings, but it is only
occasionally that we meet with cases of very
remarkable colour adaptation. Two such, however,
occur among English crab-spiders. One is a species
not uncommon in the south of England, and fairly
plentiful in the New Forest, where it is to be sought
among the lichen on the tree trunks, where its blue-grey
body, marked with black and white blotches
makes it practically invisible except when in motion.
It rejoices in the name of <i>Philodromus margaritatus</i>.
The other case is that of the spider known as
<i>Misumena vatia</i>, which is variable in colour, some
specimens being yellow and others pink, while a
variety of the species has a blood-red streak
decorating the front part of its abdomen. If it were<span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_55" id="Page_55">[Pg 55]</SPAN></span>
to choose lichen as a hunting ground there would be
little chance of concealment, but it does nothing
so foolish:—it hides among the petals of flowers,
generally, but not always, among flowers more or less
of its own colour.</p>
<p>Now this phenomenon of resemblance is sometimes
carried very much farther than a tolerable correspondence
between the colour of an animal and its
surroundings; it occasionally amounts to an apparent
imitation, in form and in behaviour as well as in
colour, of some other object, either animal or vegetable
and in such cases we have examples of what is known
as Mimicry. Most people have seen remarkable
instances of this phenomenon in the “stick” and “leaf”
insects of entomological collections. There are
several different ways in which such a resemblance
may be profitable to the imitator. Clearly it may be
advantageous for a weak animal to be mistaken for
one much more formidable and less likely to be
attacked, or for an insect which is really extremely
good eating to resemble closely one which birds well
know to be unpalatable. Or again, if your line is to
lie <i>perdu</i> and wait for some unwary insect to come
within reach, it must be a distinct asset to be indistinguishable
from such an innocent object as a twig or
a leaf; and the same disguise may serve you if you
are the possible victim and you can make the would-be
devourer believe that you are a mere vegetable.</p>
<p><span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_56" id="Page_56">[Pg 56]</SPAN></span></p>
<p>It is seldom difficult to see some such possibility
of gain in the numerous well-known cases of insect
mimicry. The wasp tribe—formidable with their
stings—are often “mimicked”; the unpalatable
Heliconid butterflies are “imitated” by members of
edible families, and some insects are such exact
imitations of leaves that the all-devouring army ants
have been seen to run over them without discovering
the imposition.</p>
<p>“Mimicry” is an unfortunate term inasmuch as
it seems to imply intentional imitation; “protective
resemblance” is better. It is generally accounted
for by the action of “natural selection” upon random
variations. No two members of a brood are exactly
alike; slight variations in form, size, colour, etc., are
constantly occurring, and when the variation is a
useful one the animal possessing it has a slightly
better chance of surviving and rearing progeny, some
of whom will probably possess the same peculiarity,
perhaps even in a more marked degree, and will be
better equipped than their neighbours in the struggle
for life. The happy possessors of such favourable
variations are thus in a sense “selected” by nature,
and this selection, acting through countless generations,
is thought to be the chief agent in bringing
about the remarkable phenomenon of protective
resemblance.</p>
<p>The theory has, no doubt, been pushed too far;<span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_57" id="Page_57">[Pg 57]</SPAN></span>
fanciful resemblances have been detected and
advantages of which there is no proof are sometimes
asserted, and moreover other possible ways
of accounting for the facts have been too much
overlooked.</p>
<p>But however it has come about, there is a case of
“mimicry” among crab-spiders which deserves more
than a passing mention. The name of the spider in
question is <i>Phrynarachne decipiens</i>, and it was
accidentally discovered by Forbes when butterfly-hunting
in Java. It spins a white patch of silk on
the upper side of a leaf on which it places itself back-downwards,
clinging to the web by means of spines
on its legs. It then folds its legs closely and lies
absolutely still. In this position the spider and web
look precisely like the dropping of some bird upon
the leaf; such droppings are frequently seen, and
seem to be particularly attractive to butterflies. It
was not until Forbes tried to catch a butterfly settled
on a leaf that he found that what looked like excrement
was really a spider which held the butterfly in
its grasp. Even after this experience he was again
deceived by the same species in Sumatra.</p>
<p>There are several extremely ant-like spiders, and
it is remarkable that some of the imitators belong to
widely different spider families:—that is to say the
resemblance has arisen independently from quite
different starting points.</p>
<p><span class="pagenum"><SPAN name="Page_58" id="Page_58">[Pg 58]</SPAN></span></p>
<p>It is very noteworthy that resemblance in structure
is always accompanied by similarity of behaviour—as
indeed it is bound to be if any benefit is to accrue
to the mimic. Your resemblance to a leaf will
deceive no one if you run wildly about, and your
imitation of an ant will lack verisimilitude if you
adopt a slow and stately method of progression.
Ant-like spiders adopt the hurried and apparently
undecided gait of their models, and insects which
look like sticks, leaves, or inanimate objects all
possess the power—and the habit—of remaining for a
long time perfectly motionless.</p>
<hr class="chap" />
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />